Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing

Carter W. Daniels, Elizabeth Watterson, Raul Garcia, Gabriel J. Mazur, Ryan J. Brackney, Federico Sanabria

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

9 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

This paper reviews the evidence for nicotine-induced acceleration of the internal clock when timing in the seconds-to-minutes timescale, and proposes an alternative explanation to this evidence: that nicotine reduces the threshold for responses that result in more reinforcement. These two hypotheses were tested in male Wistar rats using a novel timing task. In this task, rats were trained to seek food at one location after 8. s since trial onset and at a different location after 16. s. Some rats received the same reward at both times (group SAME); some received a larger reward at 16. s (group DIFF). Steady baseline performance was followed by 3 days of subcutaneous nicotine administration (0.3. mg/kg), baseline recovery, and an antagonist challenge (mecamylamine, 1.0. mg/kg). Nicotine induced a larger, immediate reduction in latencies to switch (LTS) in group DIFF than in group SAME. This effect was sustained throughout nicotine administration. Mecamylamine pretreatment and nicotine discontinuation rapidly recovered baseline performance. These results support a response-threshold account of nicotinic disruption of timing performance, possibly mediated by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. A detailed analysis of the distribution of LTSs suggests that anomalous effects of nicotine on LTS dispersion may be due to loss of temporal control of behavior.

Original languageEnglish (US)
Pages (from-to)238-250
Number of pages13
JournalBehavioural Brain Research
Volume283
DOIs
StatePublished - Apr 5 2015

Fingerprint

Nicotine
Mecamylamine
Reward
Behavior Control
Nicotinic Receptors
Wistar Rats
Food

Keywords

  • Fixed-interval
  • Mecamylamine
  • Nicotine
  • Rat
  • Time perception
  • Timing

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Behavioral Neuroscience

Cite this

Daniels, C. W., Watterson, E., Garcia, R., Mazur, G. J., Brackney, R. J., & Sanabria, F. (2015). Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing. Behavioural Brain Research, 283, 238-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.027

Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing. / Daniels, Carter W.; Watterson, Elizabeth; Garcia, Raul; Mazur, Gabriel J.; Brackney, Ryan J.; Sanabria, Federico.

In: Behavioural Brain Research, Vol. 283, 05.04.2015, p. 238-250.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Daniels, CW, Watterson, E, Garcia, R, Mazur, GJ, Brackney, RJ & Sanabria, F 2015, 'Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing', Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 283, pp. 238-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.027
Daniels, Carter W. ; Watterson, Elizabeth ; Garcia, Raul ; Mazur, Gabriel J. ; Brackney, Ryan J. ; Sanabria, Federico. / Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing. In: Behavioural Brain Research. 2015 ; Vol. 283. pp. 238-250.
@article{6156d43e74e64793b62a5aa854f3f3f4,
title = "Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing",
abstract = "This paper reviews the evidence for nicotine-induced acceleration of the internal clock when timing in the seconds-to-minutes timescale, and proposes an alternative explanation to this evidence: that nicotine reduces the threshold for responses that result in more reinforcement. These two hypotheses were tested in male Wistar rats using a novel timing task. In this task, rats were trained to seek food at one location after 8. s since trial onset and at a different location after 16. s. Some rats received the same reward at both times (group SAME); some received a larger reward at 16. s (group DIFF). Steady baseline performance was followed by 3 days of subcutaneous nicotine administration (0.3. mg/kg), baseline recovery, and an antagonist challenge (mecamylamine, 1.0. mg/kg). Nicotine induced a larger, immediate reduction in latencies to switch (LTS) in group DIFF than in group SAME. This effect was sustained throughout nicotine administration. Mecamylamine pretreatment and nicotine discontinuation rapidly recovered baseline performance. These results support a response-threshold account of nicotinic disruption of timing performance, possibly mediated by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. A detailed analysis of the distribution of LTSs suggests that anomalous effects of nicotine on LTS dispersion may be due to loss of temporal control of behavior.",
keywords = "Fixed-interval, Mecamylamine, Nicotine, Rat, Time perception, Timing",
author = "Daniels, {Carter W.} and Elizabeth Watterson and Raul Garcia and Mazur, {Gabriel J.} and Brackney, {Ryan J.} and Federico Sanabria",
year = "2015",
month = "4",
day = "5",
doi = "10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.027",
language = "English (US)",
volume = "283",
pages = "238--250",
journal = "Behavioural Brain Research",
issn = "0166-4328",
publisher = "Elsevier",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - Revisiting the effect of nicotine on interval timing

AU - Daniels, Carter W.

AU - Watterson, Elizabeth

AU - Garcia, Raul

AU - Mazur, Gabriel J.

AU - Brackney, Ryan J.

AU - Sanabria, Federico

PY - 2015/4/5

Y1 - 2015/4/5

N2 - This paper reviews the evidence for nicotine-induced acceleration of the internal clock when timing in the seconds-to-minutes timescale, and proposes an alternative explanation to this evidence: that nicotine reduces the threshold for responses that result in more reinforcement. These two hypotheses were tested in male Wistar rats using a novel timing task. In this task, rats were trained to seek food at one location after 8. s since trial onset and at a different location after 16. s. Some rats received the same reward at both times (group SAME); some received a larger reward at 16. s (group DIFF). Steady baseline performance was followed by 3 days of subcutaneous nicotine administration (0.3. mg/kg), baseline recovery, and an antagonist challenge (mecamylamine, 1.0. mg/kg). Nicotine induced a larger, immediate reduction in latencies to switch (LTS) in group DIFF than in group SAME. This effect was sustained throughout nicotine administration. Mecamylamine pretreatment and nicotine discontinuation rapidly recovered baseline performance. These results support a response-threshold account of nicotinic disruption of timing performance, possibly mediated by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. A detailed analysis of the distribution of LTSs suggests that anomalous effects of nicotine on LTS dispersion may be due to loss of temporal control of behavior.

AB - This paper reviews the evidence for nicotine-induced acceleration of the internal clock when timing in the seconds-to-minutes timescale, and proposes an alternative explanation to this evidence: that nicotine reduces the threshold for responses that result in more reinforcement. These two hypotheses were tested in male Wistar rats using a novel timing task. In this task, rats were trained to seek food at one location after 8. s since trial onset and at a different location after 16. s. Some rats received the same reward at both times (group SAME); some received a larger reward at 16. s (group DIFF). Steady baseline performance was followed by 3 days of subcutaneous nicotine administration (0.3. mg/kg), baseline recovery, and an antagonist challenge (mecamylamine, 1.0. mg/kg). Nicotine induced a larger, immediate reduction in latencies to switch (LTS) in group DIFF than in group SAME. This effect was sustained throughout nicotine administration. Mecamylamine pretreatment and nicotine discontinuation rapidly recovered baseline performance. These results support a response-threshold account of nicotinic disruption of timing performance, possibly mediated by nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. A detailed analysis of the distribution of LTSs suggests that anomalous effects of nicotine on LTS dispersion may be due to loss of temporal control of behavior.

KW - Fixed-interval

KW - Mecamylamine

KW - Nicotine

KW - Rat

KW - Time perception

KW - Timing

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84923050921&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=84923050921&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.027

DO - 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.027

M3 - Article

VL - 283

SP - 238

EP - 250

JO - Behavioural Brain Research

JF - Behavioural Brain Research

SN - 0166-4328

ER -